Parkinson's Disease Tulip


advertisement
Reply
 
Thread Tools Display Modes
Old 07-18-2007, 11:19 AM #11
paula_w paula_w is offline
In Remembrance
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: Florida
Posts: 3,904
15 yr Member
paula_w paula_w is offline
In Remembrance
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: Florida
Posts: 3,904
15 yr Member
Default Have scientists prove it

The only placebo control I ever "heard" about DBS (not verified) was in Europe somewhere they "pretended" to turn on the stimulator. Don't quote me.

That is one of the biggest points about sham surgery - the one successful surgery we have requires "patient observation" - not just statistics.

Many of us would like to know exactly how much time would be saved or lost, to just give the treatment to everyone and then observe who it works for.

Scientists should have the burden of proof that the placebo effect can last as long as they claim it can. They claimed years with GDNF.

Paula
__________________
paula

"Time is not neutral for those who have pd or for those who will get it."
paula_w is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote

advertisement
Old 07-18-2007, 10:57 PM #12
Stitcher's Avatar
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
Stitcher's Avatar
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Default

I will reply to this thread at length by the end of the weekend, but my son has to return to Iraq and I am part of that departure plan.
__________________
You're alive. Do something. The directive in life, the moral imperative was so uncomplicated. It could be expressed in single words, not complete sentences. It sounded like this: Look. Listen. Choose. Act. ~~Barbara Hall

I long to accomplish a great and noble tasks, but it is my chief duty to accomplish humble tasks as though they were great and noble. The world is moved along, not only by the mighty shoves of its heroes, but also by the aggregate of the tiny pushes of each honest worker. ~~Helen Keller
Stitcher is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 04:00 AM #13
Howardh's Avatar
Howardh Howardh is offline
Member
 
Join Date: Sep 2006
Location: New Zealand
Posts: 229
15 yr Member
Howardh Howardh is offline
Member
Howardh's Avatar
 
Join Date: Sep 2006
Location: New Zealand
Posts: 229
15 yr Member
Default We are

the pioneer generation on the NEW fronteirs in science. And whether we agree or not with the regulatory system involved in having these new innovative treatments ratified, the trials will forge ahead. Like the pioneers--in many fields-- before us the risks are huge, and those that volunteer are brave hero's who know the consequences. We are aware of the risks involved in another fronteir in science, and that is space travel. As we go beyond the outer limits of planet earth into the infinite unknowns of the universe we are aware of the lives that have been lost and the success and progress that has been made. Astronauts will continue to evolve as the universe, the galaxies beyond, and all the stars trillions of light years into the future will continue to attract us to explore. The same will apply to curing neurological diseases of the brain, huge risk will be part of the process if we are to advance and succeed.

Everything about crossing new fronteirs in any indeavour must not be caterogised as a sham, we do a disservice to the pioneers who volunteer in my view. Technical advances in science, and that includes medical means the risks are minimized and I am in support of the scientists who bring us this new hope and the volunteer trial patients who put themselves at the forefront, so as to provide the rest of us with the eventual cure.

Kudos to all those involved in bringing success to all neurological dissorders.

GO HARD.............SCIENCE
Howardh is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 05:01 AM #14
aftermathman aftermathman is offline
Member
 
Join Date: Sep 2006
Location: Evesham, England
Posts: 598
15 yr Member
aftermathman aftermathman is offline
Member
 
Join Date: Sep 2006
Location: Evesham, England
Posts: 598
15 yr Member
Default Paula, I share your disgust at "sham" however ...

imho GDNF taught us that the trouble with the placebo effect is that so little is known of it, that we are in some respects "at its mercy" in the context of clinical trials. We have to rule it out and double blind is the most effective way of doing this.

Assuming we all want to get drugs to market in the quickest way, would the process be speeded up by arguments over whether improvements are placebo or not, and if so how long the effect lasts. To get realistic data on placebo can itself take years and may itself involve sham surgery.

The Ceregene and Spheramine trials clearly state that some participants will receive sham, nothing is hidden. Steffi, I understand your strength of feeling but your post seemed to me a bit harsh. These are generally well meaning people trying to help us, who are following the clearly stated legally binding regulatory procedures currently in force. (Note this comment doesn't apply to the UK taxman with whom your analogy is probably appropriate )

Take care all, this is an important thread imho as we must ensure transparency and proper controls of sham, this will help us all get the "stuff" to market quicker.

Neil.
aftermathman is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 07:11 AM #15
reverett123's Avatar
reverett123 reverett123 is offline
In Remembrance
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 3,772
15 yr Member
reverett123 reverett123 is offline
In Remembrance
reverett123's Avatar
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 3,772
15 yr Member
Default Is it all a sham or all a shame?

The assumption is that either the current version of the scientific method is the best way or the only way to gain knowledge, and t'ain't necessarily so.

The use of sham surgery, double blind studies, etc evolved from a world where the rat was expendable. On one level we are rats in this system.

Given a population with a degenerative disease and you test a new treatment by intentionally condemning half of them, aren't the ethics questionable? If you start with a thousand aren't you precluding 500 from any chance of a life? The rat gets only one chance.

What if you gave all 1000 the chance? Don't you already have some idea of what you think will happen? If not, then you are worse than a street corner faith healer. If so, then you should be able to project an improvement of "x" percent. Why not use that as the benchmark?

And "placebo"? Many researchers almost say it with a sneer. Yet, like"idiopathic", it is the medical equivalent of "here there be dragons" on an ancient chart of a flat earth.

If you want this rat you are going to have to treat him like a man. Lay out the evidence, show me the projections, assure me that I am not going to be "sacrificed" for some procedural requirement.

The scientific method is great in the abstract but it is like religion. Depends on how you apply it.
__________________
Born in 1953, 1st symptoms and misdiagnosed as essential tremor in 1992. Dx with PD in 2000.
Currently (2011) taking 200/50 Sinemet CR 8 times a day + 10/100 Sinemet 3 times a day. Functional 90% of waking day but fragile. Failure at exercise but still trying. Constantly experimenting. Beta blocker and ACE inhibitor at present. Currently (01/2013) taking ldopa/carbadopa 200/50 CR six times a day + 10/100 form 3 times daily. Functional 90% of day. Update 04/2013: L/C 200/50 8x; Beta Blocker; ACE Inhib; Ginger; Turmeric; Creatine; Magnesium; Potassium. Doing well.
reverett123 is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 08:36 AM #16
Stitcher's Avatar
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
Stitcher's Avatar
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Default

A PWP friend of several years has been visiting for a few days. She was here to help me with my first post-op visit which had to be completed off meds. As she drove away last night she had tears in her eyes and thanked me for what I have done for her and all PWPs by participating in the CERE-120 trial, in the attempt to find a cure for this disgusting disease we all must endure daily for the rest of our lives.

Science has never come easy. It is filled with risks, whether it is testing a new vaccine for the first time or traversing the galaxies as astronauts in search of whatever they search for out there in the dark void of space. It isn’t easy and never will be. As I sit here this morning typing away on this reply, and having just returned from Philly yesterday evening, I find it hard to believe that I may have been shammed. My scores were significant yesterday. My tremor is still present, but is it far less than it has been in many years. If this is a placebo effect, I will keep it!! But if it is from the “real thing” then yippee…are we another step closer to a “cure” for this crappy disease…maybe so. Did I take a sham risk for the sake of everyone in this thread and in the PD community as a whole. Yes.

As Howard stated above, “we are the pioneers” of our PD generations. The author of this thread took a very pioneering leap of faith a few years ago by being one of the original six Spheramine recipients in a Phase I trial…talk about unknown…yes, I know it did not include a sham component, but Phase II has included sham. Some of us are willing to take the risks that come with pioneering, some are not. In either case, it is a personal decision. Sham surgery is deemed by many physicians as necessary to “view” the placebo effect, which can be. It is entirely possible that after a time sham may be abandoned as unnecessary.

Did I take the sham component of CERE-120 lightly…NO!! But, the day came when I had to make a decision to take the risk involved or not. I had to ask myself, do I continue with this decision and why. The why was a selfish choice. I knew that my only option at this point was DBS…hardware, battery change surgeries, programming, risk of infection and hardware removal, the possibility of having to endure the surgery a second time to replace what was removed, etc. Now some might say, but DBS is a known and that taking that risk is a no-brainer…no sham involved. Okay, so that makes it okay? It is still an invasion to the brain, with the risk of infection greater than CERE-120. The risk of infection with DBS isn’t just the brain, but the other parts of the body. My personal reaction could have also happened with DBS…seizure from brain swelling. Would I do the CERE-120…absolutely!

Do those of you here in this thread think that sham has never been used in other surgical settings.

The Connection.org, Show Originally Aired: 7/16/2002

http://www.theconnection.org/shows/2...716_b_main.asp

“Sham ops on arthritic knees are as successful as the real thing, which is to say, neither works at all. Faked surgery has some ethicists worried. It's one thing to hand out candy, quite another to slice into flesh on a hunch that it's worthless, exposing patients to the dangers of anesthetics and infections. But finding out what really works and what's wishful thinking could save money, time, and misery for the many creaky-jointed amongst us.”

Will we ever get passed either of the two views below…both valid. Hopefully Not! This topic…sham surgery…needs to stay of the table of discussion.

University of South Florida, Sham surgery may put patients at risk
http://student.bmj.com/back_issues/0300/news/52a.html

“Researchers carrying out sham surgery have pointed out that all clinical trials have been approved by review boards at bodies such as the US National Institutes of Health and the Food and Drug Administration. They also argue that subjects in the trials have decided to participate and that preventing patients from deciding what risks they are willing to take is a direct violation of the principle of autonomy.

“But many researchers are unhappy with the use of sham surgery in finding a treatment for Parkinson's disease. Dr Ruth Maklin of the Albert Einstein College of Medicine in New York has voiced her opposition to the surgery in the New England Journal of Medicine. "Performing a surgical procedure that has no expected benefit other than the placebo effect violates the ethical and regulatory principal that the risk of harm to subjects must be minimised in the conduct of research," said Dr Maklin.”

Sham Surgery Returns as a Research Tool
By SHERYL GAY STOLBERG, April 1999
http://www.nytimes.com/library/revie...cs-review.html

“In 1939, long before high-tech drugs came along to treat the chest pain known as angina, an Italian surgeon named Fieschi devised a simple technique. Reasoning that increased blood flow to the heart would ease his patients' pain, he made tiny incisions in their chests and tied knots in two arteries. The results were spectacular. Three quarters of all patients improved." One third were cured.

“Two decades later, the National Institutes of Health paid a young cardiologist in Seattle, Dr. Leonard A. Cobb, to conduct a novel test of the Fieschi technique. Cobb operated on 17 patients. Eight had their arteries tied; the other nine got incisions, nothing more. In 1959, the New England Journal of Medicine published his findings: The phony operations worked just as well as the real thing.

“That was beginning of the end of the procedure, known as internal mammary artery ligation; within two years, it became a footnote in the medical history texts. It was also the beginning and the end, apparently, for sham surgery in this country. By the early 1970s, an ethics revolution had transformed human experiments, and the idea of surgery as placebo was unthinkable.

“Unthinkable until now. Sham surgery is on the rise, to the horror of some doctors who say it is immoral and to the delight of others who say it is time to apply the same rigorous scientific standards to surgery as to the rest of the medical profession. And with it comes a host of thorny questions.

“The results of the first sham brain surgery study were reported last week in Toronto by a team of neurologists. Forty people with Parkinson's disease participated. Each had neurosurgery: four tiny holes, drilled through the forehead into the skull. But only half got the injections of fetal cells that might have repaired their damaged brains; the other half got nothing. One year later, three members of the placebo group said their symptoms had improved.”

Sham surgery can be ethical and safe
http://jnnp.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/74/2/202
"Sham surgery is justified in clinical trials in Parkinson’s disease provided strict criteria are followed to ensure that it is used safely and effectively, according to an article which defends the practice in judging outcome of fetal tissue implantation treatment.

"Sham surgery is controversial and used rarely. Its recent use in two trials assessing treatment for Parkinson’s disease has, naturally enough, provoked much ethical debate. According to bioethicist Ruth Macklin, the concept of sham surgery controls produces, "tension between the highest standards of research and the highest standards of ethics." Other critics see the bigger picture, recognising that clinical trials need to benefit patients, society, and research, though still opposing sham surgery as unnecessary.

"The article’s author, a neurologist, emphasises the paramount need to exclude false positive results in these trials in the interests of patient and public safety. In Parkinson’s disease this need can be fulfilled only by sham surgery controls, he argues, because of the significant and sustained placebo effect reported in medical clinical trials and its confounding of subjective outcomes so often relied on for assessing treatment. A recently published trial using sham surgery controls in the United States showed that despite promising initial results the long term results differed little between treatment and control arms because of the significant surgical placebo effect. "The recent experience with intracerebral fetal tissue grafting for Parkinson’s disease suggests that sham surgery controls can be done in a safe and ethical manner."

Full Article: http://jme.bmj.com/cgi/content/full/...665fdbf66171c3
__________________
You're alive. Do something. The directive in life, the moral imperative was so uncomplicated. It could be expressed in single words, not complete sentences. It sounded like this: Look. Listen. Choose. Act. ~~Barbara Hall

I long to accomplish a great and noble tasks, but it is my chief duty to accomplish humble tasks as though they were great and noble. The world is moved along, not only by the mighty shoves of its heroes, but also by the aggregate of the tiny pushes of each honest worker. ~~Helen Keller
Stitcher is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 09:49 AM #17
paula_w paula_w is offline
In Remembrance
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: Florida
Posts: 3,904
15 yr Member
paula_w paula_w is offline
In Remembrance
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: Florida
Posts: 3,904
15 yr Member
Default

Carolyn I'm so glad to hear that your scores have improved whether or not you have the Cere 120 - relief is relief! Not to mention seeing you post rather than brush your teeth all day -

So many good points here. Having sham surgery, like anything else that involves risk of that nature, is an individual decision. I think it's important to know all of the facts before accepting or condemning it, which is one of the issues that Pipeline is attempting to work on right now.

Aftermathman, I understand completely what you are saying. We don't want to waste time. But perhaps just reverse it and give everyone the treatment, but don't tell them they all got it? That sounds too easy so I guess it isn't a valid approach - I'm no statistician.

Rick I also understand your point - the human element has been missing and needs to be included - to the point of a whole new paradigm.

Howard sham surgery wasn't a name we chose - wonder who did choose it? It doesn't do much for promoting it by calling it sham.

Regardless of all else said, we all agree that people who participate in these trials have our complete admiration and gratefulness.

Glad you are back Carolyn, and Dottie, we'd love to hear from you too!

Paula
__________________
paula

"Time is not neutral for those who have pd or for those who will get it."
paula_w is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 10:39 AM #18
jeanb's Avatar
jeanb jeanb is offline
Senior Member
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: sonoran desert
Posts: 1,352
15 yr Member
jeanb jeanb is offline
Senior Member
jeanb's Avatar
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Location: sonoran desert
Posts: 1,352
15 yr Member
Exclamation Thank you Carolyn & Dottie & Todd & ...

Carolyn, it is so good to hear from you and have you back with us!

Carolyn is my friend, and she knows that I have supported her in this decision since the beginning. And it was with a heavy heart that I followed her struggles after her surgery.

I suspect we are all united in hating the idea of sham surgery. I wish it were not part of the CERE trial. But it IS and this is the only way this substance is being tested. Without people like Carolyn and Todd and Dottie (and all the rest) this treatment will never reach all PWP - assuming that this and succeeding trials are a success.

Participating in clinical trials is an individual matter of conscience. However I think it is a shame that so few pwp believe in the process - only 1% of PWP in the US participate in clinical trials. That slows down the time to get treatments or a cure for all of us.

Have I participated in clinical trials? Yes - 6 and counting. Have any involved brain surgery - no - i have not been eligible. But asssuming I AM eligible for CERE phase III, I'll be in line to sign up - sham surgery or no.

I will never like the idea of sham surgery, but if it is the only way this and other promising treatments will make it through the drug pipeline, I'll take my chances. And I'll be proud to have participated in a process that helps all PWP.
__________________
Jean B

This isn't the life I wished for, but it is the life I have. So I'm doing my best.
jeanb is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 01:12 PM #19
AnnT2 AnnT2 is offline
Member
 
Join Date: Oct 2006
Posts: 148
15 yr Member
AnnT2 AnnT2 is offline
Member
 
Join Date: Oct 2006
Posts: 148
15 yr Member
Default

Hi Carolyn -
Thank you for addressing my question about whether other organs have been invaded or accessed for a sham operation, thus providing a group for those receiving the real treatment to compare results with.

Still, why can't they just compare people having the surgery with like people not having the surgery? Is the placebo effect so powerful that it can sustain an otherwise untreated patient in a healthy state for a significant period of time?

Thank you also for going through this operation. I hope everyone receiving the actual therapy does well. Meanwhile, it scares me to contemplate it for myself. Your bravery is admirable.

Ann
AnnT2 is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Old 07-19-2007, 02:22 PM #20
Stitcher's Avatar
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Stitcher Stitcher is offline
Magnate
Stitcher's Avatar
 
Join Date: Aug 2006
Posts: 2,136
15 yr Member
Default The Power of Placebo

Listen - New York Public Radio
http://www.wnyc.org/shows/radiolab/episodes/2007/05/18

lines_red_039.jpg

Benefiting from Belief
http://www.fda.gov/fdac/features/2000/100_heal.html

Researchers have been studying the placebo effect for decades. In 1955, researcher H.K. Beecher published his groundbreaking paper "The Powerful Placebo," in which he concluded that, across the 26 studies he analyzed, an average of 32 percent of patients responded to placebo. In the 1960s, breakthrough studies showed the potential physiological effects of dummy pills--they tended to speed up pulse rate, increase blood pressure, and improve reaction speeds, for example, when participants were told they had taken a stimulant, and had the opposite physiological effects when participants were told they had taken a sleep-producing drug.

Yet, even after 40 years, big questions remain about the interplay of psychological and physiological mechanisms that contribute to the placebo effect. Today's brain imagery techniques do lend support, though, to the theory that thoughts and beliefs not only affect one's psychological state, but also cause the body to undergo actual biological changes.

The phenomenon needn't baffle us, says Michael Jospe, a professor at the California School of Professional Psychology who has studied the placebo effect for more than 20 years. He points out that all people experience physiological reactions to anticipation and stress--something like the fight-or-flight response--that help them to survive and cope. When you step out of your office and a spider jumps out at you, Jospe analogizes, "you'll get a fright and have a physiological reaction. And the next time you go out that way, the thought that it could happen again can produce a physiological reaction before you even open the door." So, he says, the relationship between a thought and a negative psychophysiological reaction like fear is something we experience daily.

That goes for positive associations, too, Jospe continues. "The placebo effect is part of the human potential to react positively to a healer. You can reduce a patient's distress by doing something which might not be medically effective." It's like kids and Band-Aids, Jospe says. "When you put a Band-Aid on a child and it has stars or comics on it, it can actually make the kid feel better by its soothing effect, though there's no medical reason it should make the child feel better."

There is no medical reason, either, that look-alike placebo tablets used in a 1997 study of benign enlargement of the prostate gland should have made the study participants feel better. But in this Canadian study, more than half of the men who got the placebo pills reported significant relief from their symptoms, including faster urine flow. Researcher J. Curtis Nickel theorized that the patients' positive expectations of the experimental drug's benefits may have caused therapeutic smooth muscle relaxation by decreasing nerve activity affecting the bladder, prostate and urethra. Study participants on placebo complained of side effects, too (sometimes called the "nocebo" effect), ranging from impotence and reduced sex drive to nausea, diarrhea and constipation.

It's this powerful placebo effect, coupled with the fact that many medical conditions involve a natural course of better and worse periods (arthritis and multiple sclerosis are examples of diseases with flair-ups and lulls), that can make it difficult to know if a health upswing should be credited to a drug effect. One way to account for such variables in a drug study: give one group of patients placebo and another the experimental drug, and see if the drug group's health improvements sufficiently surpass those from placebo. In Straus' study, the chronic fatigue syndrome drug failed to adequately demonstrate its superiority over dummy pills.

Proof in the Placebo

FDA doesn't require that a drug study include a placebo control group, DeLap says, only that its design be capable of establishing a drug's safety and effectiveness. Non-placebo types of drug studies include "head-to-head" studies, which compare the experimental drug to an existing treatment, and historically controlled studies, which compare the new drug's effects with information gathered in the past about the expected progression of a medical condition.

Often, however, a placebo control can provide the clearest insight into what a treatment can accomplish, according to DeLap, especially with some psychiatric and other drugs in which the placebo effect is known to play a particularly weighty role. In fact, DeLap says, in some cases the placebo effect "makes it almost hopeless, statistically" to use studies that test a new treatment side-by-side against an existing one and determine whether the new treatment works.

The placebo controls that have traditionally been used to test medications have recently been used, too, to test the effectiveness of surgical procedures. In one well-publicized study sponsored by the National Institutes of Health, half of the Parkinson's disease patients enrolled in the trial underwent a sham surgery in which doctors drilled holes into their skulls but didn't implant the potentially beneficial human fetal tissue in their brains.

While FDA doesn't evaluate the safety or effectiveness of most surgical techniques, the agency does regulate surgical implantation of animal cells or re-engineered human tissues. The agency has approved at least one sham surgery-controlled trial, which will study the effectiveness of implanted pig fetal cells for Parkinson's.

Even with the powerful scientific advantages of including a placebo control, researchers and FDA must look at each treatment individually to decide if the use of placebos is appropriate and ethical. In fact, much medical research does not involve a placebo control because "it's just not an option, ethically," DeLap says.

To determine whether a placebo-controlled trial is acceptable, drug company and FDA experts ask: For what condition is this drug being tested? What is the natural progression of the disease? How serious is the risk if a patient gets a placebo rather than an active treatment?

In DeLap's specialty, oncology, placebo-controlled studies are often unacceptable because of the great risk to cancer patients of any treatment delays. For a headache, on the other hand, patients in a study may be uncomfortable for a time, but are not at risk of a lasting health impact. So, for those conditions in which the downside of being on placebo is modest and short-lived, DeLap says, it's an individual's prerogative to say, "I know what I'm getting into, and I want to further this scientific research."

To help ensure that patients know the pros and cons of enrolling in a study, each participant must sign an "informed consent" form, which clearly explains:
  • the purpose of the study
  • what enrollees will be asked to do (take a pill twice a day for three months, for example, and visit the doctor once a week for blood and other laboratory tests)
  • the possible benefits and known adverse reactions associated with the experimental treatment
  • other therapies that are available for their condition.
Even willing participants can't sign away their right to a well-designed study, though, DeLap points out. "We can't fall into the trap of thinking that, once someone says 'I'm willing to participate,' their consent covers us for deficiencies. Our responsibilities go way beyond getting informed consent." One such responsibility: As a study progresses, researchers monitor results so if major positive or negative drug effects are seen, the study can be stopped. The first major clinical study of the AIDS drug AZT (zidovudine), for example, was halted early when researchers saw that AZT patients were living significantly longer than others in the study.

Still, some critics say today's safeguards are not sufficient and oppose the use of placebo-controlled studies in almost all drug research. Patients desperately seeking to end their suffering, some say, may not be capable of giving true informed consent.

DeLap and other FDA experts view any far-reaching ban on placebos in research as paternalistic. "We at FDA don't have an ethical blind spot, as some would suggest," DeLap says. "A patient's right to the best treatment is always paramount. But the social hope is that careful scientific research can help us learn beyond a shadow of a doubt what works and what doesn't, so that these patients' kids will have better treatments available to them."

Placebos in the Doctor's Office: Opposing Views

Today, when 26-year-old Jennifer Kennedy (not her real name) from Rockville, Md., has a panic attack, she simply distracts herself with a book or crossword puzzle. But at their height, several years ago, the attacks scared her so much that she finally went to the emergency room. "I thought I was going to die from a heart attack," says Kennedy.

She didn't believe her doctor when he told her to "just relax" and she would feel okay, and he ended up prescribing pills. He didn't tell her until two weeks later, after her panic attacks had subsided, that the pills were simply placebos with no active medical ingredient.

Those who oppose the use of placebo pills in medical practice say that such deceit can undermine the essential trust between patient and doctor. Gastroenterologist Michael Kirsch, M.D., has called doctors who prescribe placebos outside medical research "con artists." Kirsch asks in a 1998 editorial in Priorities, the magazine of the nonprofit American Council on Science and Health, "If using placebos therapeutically is ethical and reasonable, shouldn't we encourage judges to render extralegal activist rulings, winegrowers to bottle 'placebo' vintages, curators to display masterpiece look-alikes misleadingly, and journalists and newscasters to sanitize news?" He continues, "In such a world, all of us would be groping for truth in a hall of mirrors."

But placebo researcher Michael Jospe disagrees with what he calls this "strict, grumpy approach that concludes that any doctor who uses placebos is acting unethically." Some circumstances, he says, justify this kind of benevolent deception--like when a patient insists on a medicine that is unnecessary and carries needless risks.

"You've got to be there on the oncology ward," Jospe says, "and see how suffering people get so demanding of drugs that might be extremely harmful to them. If you look at sugar pills in the broader context of a supportive doctor-patient relationship rather than just as ripping off the patient, you may come to a different conclusion" about the ethics of placebos.

It's not uncommon for a patient to feel betrayed initially upon hearing that they were given a placebo, Jospe says, but a sensitive doctor can explain to the patient, "No, that the placebo worked doesn't mean you're crazy. You were just in distress and thus more prone to reacting to anything with the potential to help."

Kennedy admits to feeling deceived when she first found out the pills her doctor prescribed were fake, but says she now appreciates the doctor's decision to prescribe the placebo. "At first, I felt stupid. But that day at the hospital, the doctor must have realized I wasn't going to accept 'you're fine, it's all in your mind.' The placebo helped me realize that I'm not unhealthy and I'm going to be okay. Now, I think it's really neat that something that didn't really affect me had such a big effect on my life."

lines_red_039.jpg

The Placebo Effect: The Triumph of Mind over Body

http://www.nexusmagazine.com/article...eboEffect.html
A growing body of research reveals not just psychological and perceptual components to the placebo effect but also a biochemical substrate to the mechanism.

Extracted from Nexus Magazine, Volume 14, Number 4 (June - July 2007)
From our web page at: www.nexusmagazine.com

by Peter Arguriou © 2007

A neglected phenomenon

One of the most commonly used terms in medical language is the word placebo. The placebo effect is used as a scale for evaluating the effectiveness of new drugs. But what exactly is the placebo effect and what are its consequences in the deterministic structure of Western medicine?
The placebo effect has been frequently abused by health professionals to denote and stigmatise a fraud or fallacy. Alternative therapies have often been characterised as merely placebos. But the placebo effect is not a fraudulent, useless or malevolent phenomenon. It occurs independently of the intentions of charlatans or health professionals. It is a spontaneous, authentic and very factual phenomenon that refers to well-observed but uninterpreted and contingent therapies or health improvements that occur in the absence of an active chemical/pharmacological substance. Make-believe drugs-drugs that carry no active chemical substances-often act as the real drugs and provoke therapeutic effects when administered to patients.

In many drug trials, the manufacturers of the drug sadly discover that their product is in no way superior to the effect of a placebo. But that does not mean that a placebo equates to a null response of the human organism. On the contrary, a placebo denotes non-chemical stimuli that strongly motivate the organism towards a therapeutic course. That is, the placebo effect is dependent not on the drug's effectiveness but solely on therapeutic intention and expectation.

Effects of positive and negative thinking
The placebo effect has been often misunderstood as a solely psychological and highly subjective phenomenon. The patient, convinced of the therapy's effectiveness, ignores his symptoms or perceives them faintly without any substantial improvement of his health; that is, the patient feels better but is not healthier. But can the subjective psychological aspect of the placebo effect account for all of its therapeutic properties? The answer is definite: the placebo effect refers to an alternative curative mechanism that is inherent in the human entity, is motivated by therapeutic intention or belief in the therapeutic potential of a treatment, and implies biochemical responses and reactions to the stimulus of therapeutic intention or belief.

But placebos are not always beneficial: they can also have adverse effects. For example, administering a pharmacologically inactive substance to some patients can sometimes bring about unexpected health deteriorations. A review of 109 double-blind studies estimated that 19 per cent of placebo recipients manifested the nocebo effect: unexpected deteriorations of health.1 In a related experiment, researchers falsely declared to the volunteers that a weak electrical current would pass through their head; although there was no electrical current, 70 per cent of the volunteers (who were medical students) complained of a headache after the experiment.
In a group of patients suffering from carotid atherosclerosis, prognosis and progression of the disease were burdened when their psychological health was bad (i.e., they were affected by hopelessness or depression). In another group of carotid atherosclerosis patients, prognosis and progression were burdened not only by hopelessness but also by hostility. In patients with coronary heart disease, hopelessness was a determinative risk factor.4 Social isolation, work stress and hostility comprised additional risk factors.5
Positive or negative thinking seems to be a decisive risk factor for every treatment, perhaps even more important than medical intervention.
The nocebo effect appears to have a specific biological substrate. A group of 15 men whose wives suffered from terminal cancer participated in a small perspective study. After their wives' deaths, the men experienced severe grief that caused immunodepression. The spouses' lymphocytes for a period of time after their wives' deaths responded poorly to mitogenes. Grief had assaulted their immune system. The study proposed that grief and grief-induced immunodepression resulted in high- level mortality of the specific group.

A short history of a small miracle
The term placebo (meaning "I shall please") was used in mediaeval prayer in the context of the phrase Placebo Domino ("I shall please the Lord") and originated from a biblical translation of the fifth century AD.7 During the 18th century, the term was adopted by medicine and was used to imply preparations of no therapeutic value that were administered to patients as "decoy drugs". The term began to transform in 1920 (Graves8), and through various intermediate stages (Evans and Hoyle, 19339; Gold, Kwit and Otto, 193710; Jellinek, 194611) was fully transformed in 1955 when it finally claimed an important portion of the therapeutic effect in general. Henry K. Beecher, in his 1955 paper "The Powerful Placebo", attributed a rough percentage of 30 per cent of the overall therapeutic benefit to the placebo effect.
In certain later studies, the placebo effect was estimated at even higher, at 60 per cent of the overall therapeutic outcome. In a recent review of 39 studies regarding the effectiveness of antidepressant drugs, psychologist Guy Sapirstein concluded that 50 per cent of the therapeutic benefits came from the placebo effect, with a poor percentage of 27 per cent attributed to drug intervention (fluoxetine, sertaline and paroxetine). Three years later Sapirstein, along with a fellow psychologist Irving Kirsch, processed the data from 19 double-blind studies regarding depression and reached an even higher percentage of therapeutic results attributed to the placebo effect: 75 per cent of depression therapies or ameliorations were placebo induced!

Hróbjartsson and Gotzsche (200114, 200415) doubted the effectiveness of the placebo phenomenon, attributing it solely to the subjective factors of human psychology. And indeed, there is a major aspect of the placebo effect related to psychology. In two studies where placebos were exclusively administered, the placebo effect seemed to be effected from the subject's perception of the applied therapy, i.e., two placebo pills were better than one, bigger pills were better than smaller, and injections were even better.
The placebo induced a reaction not only to the therapy but also to its form, suggesting that the placebo phenomenon is shaped according to the personal symbolic universe of the patient. Before the placebo response occurs, human perception has already interpreted the applied therapy and has prepared a certain response to it. It would appear that not only chemical but also non-chemical stimuli participate in the motivation of the human organism towards therapy.

But is the placebo reaction solely a psychological phenomenon or does it have additional tangible somatic effects?

One of the more dramatic events regarding placebo therapy was reported in 1957 when a new wonder drug, Krebiozen, held promise as the final solution to the cancer problem. A patient with metastatic tumours and with fluid collection in his lungs, who demanded the daily intake of oxygen and the use of an oxygen mask, heard of Krebiozen. His doctor was participating in Krebiozen research and the patient begged him to be given the revolutionary drug. Bent by the patient's hopelessness, the doctor did so and witnessed a miraculous recovery of the patient. His tumours melted and he returned to an almost normal lifestyle. The recovery didn't last long. The patient read articles about Krebiozen's not delivering what it promised in cancer therapy. The patient then had a relapse; his tumours were back. His doctor, deeply affected by the aggravation, resorted to a desperate trick. He told his patient that he had in his possession a new, improved version of Krebiozen. It was simply distilled water. The patient fully recovered after the placebo treatment and remained functional for two months. The final verdict on Krebiozen, published in the press, proved the drug to be totally ineffective. That was the coup de grace for the patient, who died a few days later.

In spite of the melodrama of the Krebiozen case, there is no single case or personal testimony that can denote or prove a therapy to be effective. Statistical studies, not personal testimonies, can verify a proposed therapy's effectiveness, and well-planned studies are able to concur that the placebo phenomenon has somatic properties.

One such study was implemented in 1997. The two study groups consisted of patients with benign prostatic hypertrophy. One group took actual medication while the control group received placebo treatment. The placebo recipients reported relief from their symptoms and even amelioration of their urinary function.18 A placebo has also been reported to act as a bronchodilator in asthmatic patients, or to have the exact opposite action-respiratory depression-depending on the description of the pharmacological effect the researchers gave to the patients and therefore the effect the patients anticipated.

A placebo proved highly efficient against food allergies and, subsequently, impressively effective in the sinking of biotechnologies on the stockmarket. How could that be? Peptide Therapeutics Group, a biotech company, was preparing to launch on the market a novel vaccine for food allergies. The first reports were encouraging. When the experimental vaccine reached the clinical trials stage, the company's spokesperson boasted that the vaccine proved effective in 75 per cent of the cases-a percentage that usually suffices to prove a drug's effectiveness. But the good news didn't last long. The control group, given a placebo, did almost as well: seven out of 10 patients reported getting rid of their food allergies. The stock value of the company plunged by 33 per cent. The placebo effect on food allergies created a nocebo effect on the stockmarket!20 In another case, a genetically designed heart drug that raised high hopes for Genentech was clobbered by a placebo.

As aptly put by science historian Anne Harrington, placebos are "ghosts that haunt our house of biomedical objectivity and expose the paradoxes and fissures in our own self-created definitions of the real and active factors in treatment".

The placebo's pharmacomimetic behaviour can even imitate a drug's side effects. In a 1997 study of patients with benign prostate hypertrophy, some patients on a placebo complained of various side effects ranging from impotence and reduced sexual activity to nausea, diarrhoea and constipation. Another study reported placebo side effects as including headaches, vomiting, nausea and a variety of other symptoms.

The placebo effect in surgery

But how deep can the placebo effect trespass into the well-defined area of medicine? Surely it can't joust with medicine's strike force; it cannot challenge surgery. Or can it?

In 1939, an Italian surgeon named Davide Fieschi invented a new technique for treating angina pectoris (chest pain due to ischaemia or lack of blood/oxygen getting to the heart muscle, usually due to obstruction of the coronary arteries).24 Reasoning that increased blood flow to the heart would reduce his patients' pain, he performed tiny incisions in their chests and tied knots on the two internal mammary arteries. Three quarters of the patients showed improvement; one quarter of them was cured. The surgical intervention became standard procedure for the treatment of angina for the next 20 years. But in 1959, a young cardiologist, Leonard Cobb, put the Fieschi procedure to the test. He operated on 17 patients: on eight of them he followed the standard procedure; on the other nine he performed only the tiny incisions, letting the patients believe that they'd had the real thing. The result was a real upset: those who'd had the sham surgery did as well as those who'd had the Fieschi technique.25 That was the end of the Fieschi technique and the beginning of the documented surgical placebo effect.
In 1994, surgeon J. Bruce Moseley experimented with the surgical placebo. He split a small group of patients suffering from osteoarthritis of the knee into two equal groups. Both groups were told that they would undergo arthroscopic surgery, but only the first group got the real thing. The other group was left virtually untreated, with the doctor performing only tiny incisions to make the arthroscopic scenario credible. Similar results were reported in both groups.

Moseley, stunned by the outcome, decided to perform the trial with a larger statistical sample in order to reach safer conclusions. The results were replicated: arthroscopic surgery was equal therapeutically to the placebo effect.27 The placebo had found its way into surgical rooms.
Perhaps the most impressive aspect of surgical placebo arose in a groundbreaking 2004 study. In the innovative field of stem-cell research, a new approach was taken with Parkinson's disease. Human embryonic dopamine neurons were implanted through tiny holes in the patients' brains. Once again, the results were encouraging. And once again, the procedure failed to do better than a placebo. In this case, the placebo involved tiny holes incised in the skull without implantation of stem cells. As the researchers confessed, "The placebo effect was very strong in this study".

But how can it be that the therapeutic expectancy alone often produces results equal to those from actual surgery? It appears that the mind is exerting control over somatic processes, including diseases. The biochemical traces of this influence are only beginning to be outlined. Modern research indicates a biological, tangible substrate to the placebo effect.

Somatic pathways
In the mid-1990s, researcher Fabrizio Benedetti conducted a novel experiment whereby he induced ischaemic pain and soothed it by administering morphine. When morphine was replaced by a saline solution, the placebo presented analgesic properties. However, when naloxone (an opiate antagonist) was added to the saline solution, the analgesic properties of the water were blocked. Benedetti reached the conclusion that the placebo's analgesic properties were a result of specific biochemical paths. Naloxone blocked not only morphine but also endogenous opioids-the physical pain-relievers.

The endogenous opioids, endorphins, were discovered in 1974 and act as pain antagonists. Benedetti's suggestion of a placebo-induced release of endorphins was supported by findings produced with MRI and PET scans.30 Placebo-induced endorphin release also affects heart rate and respiratory activity.31 As researcher Jon-Kar Zubieta described, "...this [finding] deals another serious blow to the idea that the placebo effect is a purely psychological, not physical, phenomenon".

Further findings support the notion that the placebo effect presents a biochemical substrate in both depression and Parkinson's disease. Analysing the results of PET scans, researchers estimated the glucose metabolism in the brains of patients with depression. Glucose metabolism under placebo presented differentiations that were similar to those caused by antidepressants such as fluoxetine.33 In patients suffering from Parkinson's disease, a placebo injection promoted dopamine secretion in a similar way to that caused by amphetamine administration.34 Benedetti demonstrated that the placebo effect provoked decreased activity in single neurons of the subthalamic nucleus in patients with Parkinson's disease.

From numerous research findings, it is logical and rather safe to conclude that there is a biochemical substrate to the placebo effect. But what is more intriguing to it is its relation to perception. It would appear that perception and the codes and symbols that the animate computer, the brain, utilises in order to process internal and external information strongly determine the potency and form of placebo response.

In a recent study, patients were purposely misinformed that they had been infected by hazardous bacilli and they subsequently underwent treatment. However, there were no bacilli and the treatment administered was a placebo. Guess what? Some of the study subjects developed infection-like conditions that were not treatable by the placebo medication.36 The mind interpreted the fictional bacilli as hazardous and instructed the body to respond to them as if they were real.

Despite the placebo's potency and its importance for a new perception of health where body and mind heavily interact, large numbers of scientists continue to regard the placebo as an insignificant systematic error, a troublesome nought. According to cancer researcher Gershom Zajicek: "There is nothing in the pharmacokinetic theory which accounts for the placebo effect. In order to keep the theory consistent, the placebo effect is regarded as random error or noise which can be ignored."

One of the most perceptive placebo researchers was Stewart Wolf, "the father of psychosomatic medicine", who as early as 1949 had given it a thorough description. Wolf not only defended the placebo as a non-fictional and very "real" phenomenon but also described the placebo's pharmacomimetic behaviour. He was perhaps the first researcher to correlate the placebo effect not only with psychology and predisposition but also with perception. More than half a century ago, he stated that "the mechanisms of the body are capable of reacting not only to direct physical and chemical stimulation but also to symbolic stimuli, words and events which have somehow acquired special meaning for the individual".

In this context, a pill is not merely an active substance but also a therapeutic symbol and thus the organism is able to respond not only to its chemical content but also to its symbolic content. Likewise a bacillus, beyond its physical properties, acquires symbolic properties that can cause an organism's reaction even in the absence of the bacillus.

The presence and extent of the nocebo effect should also be studied in regard to drug resistance. Perhaps drug resistance is a multifactorial phenomenon involving not only microbial evolutionary aptness but also human psyche mechanics. Placebo and nocebo phenomena might prove fundamental not only on the personal level but also in the public health arena.
They might even provide the foundation stone for a new model of health, a new medicine that was envisioned by Wolf in the 1950s: "...in the future, drugs will be assessed not only with reference to their pharmacologic action but also to the other forces at play and to the circumstances surrounding their administration".

Five centuries ago, Swiss alchemist and physician Paracelsus (1493-1541) wrote: "You must know that the will is a powerful adjuvant of medicine." It seems that our scientific arrogance has blinded us to the teachings of the past.

About the Author:
Peter Arguriou was born in Greece in the summer of 1973. He studied medicine at the University of Athens Medical School, but left disappointed by the mechanistic perceptions governing medicine. Later, he briefly studied classical homoeopathy at the Aegean University under Alternative Nobel Prize winner George Vithoulkas. He writes for the Greek press and is the author of eight books (fiction, science fiction, poetry - most of them still unpublished). He is a member of the Hellenic MENSA and currently is working on a book regarding novel epidemics, bad science, the gene promise, the media travesty in coverage of science news, orchestrated propaganda and the corruption of the scientific establishment by big business and political agendas. He can be contacted by email at petrosarguriou@hotmail.com.

lines_red_039.jpg

Source: Mayo Clinic
Dateecember 31, 2003

Placebo Effect: Harnessing Your Mind's Power To Heal

http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases...1231084101.htm

Science Daily — It's true that some people who participate in research studies and take inactive medications called placebos do see health improvements. People taking placebos have experienced reduced pain, healed ulcers, eased nausea and even warts disappeared.

The January issue of Mayo Clinic Women's HealthSource details several theories on how the placebo effect might work:

Benefit from attention: In a placebo-controlled research study, patients often have frequent and intensive medical attention. Some people respond favorably.

Stimulus response: People may have a trained positive response to taking a pill or receiving treatment, whether it's real or not.

Beliefs or expectations, including the meaning you attach to a treatment: A person with positive expectations of the treatment may experience the placebo effect more than someone with lower expectations.

Relationship with your doctor: A person whose doctor is supportive and positive may experience more benefit from a placebo -- or the standard treatment -- than someone who doesn't have that relationship.

Pleasing your doctor: You feel better because -- consciously or unconsciously -- you want to show your doctor that you're a good patient and you appreciate the care.

Probably a combination of many psychological and physiological mechanisms are at work. Research studies and theories hold important clues to solve the mystery behind the placebo effect, but more research is needed to examine how these factors interplay to produce this healing force.

Note: This story has been adapted from a news release issued by Mayo Clinic.

lines_red_039.jpg

Tuesday, December 30, 2003
Placebo Effect: Harnessing Your Mind’s Power to Heal


It’s true that some people who participate in research studies and take inactive medications called placebos do see health improvements. People taking placebos have experienced reduced pain, healed ulcers, eased nausea and even warts disappeared.

The January issue of Mayo Clinic Women’s HealthSource details several theories on how the placebo effect might work:

Benefit from attention: In a placebo-controlled research study, patients often have frequent and intensive medical attention. Some people respond favorably.

Stimulus response: People may have a trained positive response to taking a pill or receiving treatment, whether it’s real or not.

Beliefs or expectations, including the meaning you attach to a treatment: A person with positive expectations of the treatment may experience the placebo effect more than someone with lower expectations.

Relationship with your doctor: A person whose doctor is supportive and positive may experience more benefit from a placebo -- or the standard treatment -- than someone who doesn’t have that relationship.

Pleasing your doctor: You feel better because -- consciously or unconsciously -- you want to show your doctor that you’re a good patient and you appreciate the care.

Probably a combination of many psychological and physiological mechanisms are at work. Research studies and theories hold important clues to solve the mystery behind the placebo effect, but more research is needed to examine how these factors interplay to produce this healing force.

lines_red_039.jpg
__________________
You're alive. Do something. The directive in life, the moral imperative was so uncomplicated. It could be expressed in single words, not complete sentences. It sounded like this: Look. Listen. Choose. Act. ~~Barbara Hall

I long to accomplish a great and noble tasks, but it is my chief duty to accomplish humble tasks as though they were great and noble. The world is moved along, not only by the mighty shoves of its heroes, but also by the aggregate of the tiny pushes of each honest worker. ~~Helen Keller
Stitcher is offline   Reply With QuoteReply With Quote
Reply


Posting Rules
You may not post new threads
You may not post replies
You may not post attachments
You may not edit your posts

BB code is On
Smilies are On
[IMG] code is On
HTML code is Off


Similar Threads
Thread Thread Starter Forum Replies Last Post
Dual-imaging technique useful before -- and during -- brain surgery Stitcher Parkinson's Disease 0 06-10-2007 12:38 PM
Placebo Surgery / Sham Surgery Stitcher Parkinson's Disease 3 05-23-2007 08:38 AM
Brain Surgery - DBS?? frogga Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy (RSD and CRPS) 7 12-05-2006 01:48 PM


All times are GMT -5. The time now is 06:40 PM.

Powered by vBulletin • Copyright ©2000 - 2024, Jelsoft Enterprises Ltd.

vBulletin Optimisation provided by vB Optimise v2.7.1 (Lite) - vBulletin Mods & Addons Copyright © 2024 DragonByte Technologies Ltd.
 

NeuroTalk Forums

Helping support those with neurological and related conditions.

 

The material on this site is for informational purposes only,
and is not a substitute for medical advice, diagnosis or treatment
provided by a qualified health care provider.


Always consult your doctor before trying anything you read here.